What Cocktail Parties Teach Us
The Brain Is Wired to Focus on Just One Thing;
Which Tasks Are Easier to Combine
- By MELINDA BECK
Melinda Beck on Lunch Break looks at the "cocktail party
effect," in which people are able to focus on one conversation while being
aware of conversations going on around them. Researchers say we can train our
brains to maximize this kind of awareness.
You're at a party. Music is playing. Glasses are clinking. Dozens
of conversations are driving up the decibel level. Yet amid all those
distractions, you can zero in on the one conversation you want to hear.
This ability to hyper-focus on one stream of sound amid a
cacophony of others is what researchers call the "cocktail-party
effect." Now, scientists at the University of California in San Francisco
have pinpointed where that sound-editing process occurs in the brain—in the
auditory cortex just behind the ear, not in areas of higher thought. The
auditory cortex boosts some sounds and turns down others so that when the
signal reaches the higher brain, "it's as if only one person was speaking
alone," says principle investigator Edward Chang.
These findings, published in the journal Nature last week,
underscore why people aren't very good at multitasking—our brains are wired for
"selective attention" and can focus on only one thing at a time. That
innate ability has helped humans survive in a world buzzing with visual and
auditory stimulation. But we keep trying to push the limits with multitasking,
sometimes with tragic consequences. Drivers talking on cellphones, for example,
are four times as likely to get into traffic accidents as those who aren't.
Many of those accidents are due to "inattentional
blindness," in which people can, in effect, turn a blind eye to things
they aren't focusing on. Images land on our retinas and are either boosted or
played down in the visual cortex before being passed to the brain, just as the
auditory cortex filters sounds, as shown in the Nature study last week.
"It's a push-pull relationship—the more we focus on one thing, the less we
can focus on others," says Diane M. Beck, an associate professor of
psychology at the
University of Illinois.
That people can be completely oblivious to things in their
field of vision was demonstrated famously in the "Invisible Gorilla
experiment" devised at Harvard in the
1990s. Observers are shown a short video of youths tossing a basketball and
asked to count how often the ball is passed by those wearing white. Afterward,
the observers are asked several questions, including, "Did you see the
gorilla?" Typically, about half the observers failed to notice that
someone in a gorilla suit walked through the scene. They're usually
flabbergasted because they're certain they would have noticed something like
that.
"We largely see what we expect to see," says Daniel
Simons, one of the study's creators and now a professor of psychology at the
University of Illinois. As he notes in his subsequent book, "The Invisible
Gorilla" (co-authored with Christopher Chabris), the more attention a task
demands, the less attention we can pay to other things in our field of vision.
That's why pilots sometimes fail to notice obstacles on runways and
radiologists may overlook anomalies on X-rays, especially in areas they aren't
scrutinizing.
And it isn't just that sights and sounds compete for the brain's
attention. All the sensory inputs vie to become the mind's top priority.
That's the real danger of distracted driving, experts say.
"You regularly hear people say as long as your hands are on the wheel and
your eyes are on the road, you're fine. But that's not true,"
Mr. Simons
says.
2.5%
The percentage of people who can multitask efficiently. Many
more people only think they can.
Studies over the past decade at the University of Utah show that
drivers talking on hands-free cellphones are just as impaired as those on
hands-held phones because it is the conversation, not the device, that is
draining their attention. Those talking on any kind of cellphone react more
slowly and miss more traffic signals than other motorists.
"Even though your eyes are looking right at something, when
you are on the cellphone, you are not as likely to see it," says David
Strayer, a psychology professor and lead researcher. "Ninety-nine percent
of the time, it's not that critical, but that 1% could be the time a child runs
into the street,"
he adds.
Dr. Strayer's studies have also found that talking on a cellphone
is far more distracting than conversing with a passenger—since a passenger can
see the same traffic hazards and doesn't expect a steady stream of conversation
as someone on a cellphone does. Listening to the radio, to music or to a book
on tape also isn't as distracting, because it doesn't require the same level of
interaction as a conversation. But Mr. Simons notes that even drivers may miss
some details of a book on tape if their attention is focused on merging or
other complex driving tasks.
Some people can train themselves to pay extra attention to things
that are important—like police officers learn to scan crowds for faces and
conductors can listen for individual instruments within
the orchestra as a
whole.
Multitasking may cause cognitive depletion, while 'unplugging'
has restorative properties, based on early research.
And the Utah researchers have identified a rare group of
"super-taskers"—as estimated 2.5% of the population—who seem able to
attend to more than one thing with ease.
Many more people think they can effectively multitask, but they
are really shifting their attention rapidly between two things and not getting
the full effect of either, experts say.
Indeed, some college professors have barred students from bringing
laptop computers to their classrooms, even ostensibly to take notes. Dr. Beck
says she was surprised to find that some of her students were on Facebook
during her lectures—even though the course was about selective attention.
Still, she doesn't plan to crack down. "I just explained that
doing Facebook in class means you will not learn as much, which will have
consequences on the exam,"
she says.
Clearly, it is easier to combine some tasks than others. "Not
all distractions are the same," says Dr. Strayer. Things like knitting,
cleaning and working out can be done automatically while the mind is engaged
elsewhere. But doing homework and texting simultaneously isn't possible.
(Sorry, kids).
Even conversing and watching TV is difficult. "Just try
conversing with your wife while watching football. It's impossible," jokes
Mr. Simons.
PAY ATTENTION |
How to stay in the zone
• Recognize your limitations.
The brain can only fully attend to one thing at a time.
• Make your senses work
together. If you're trying to listen to someone in a noisy room, look
directly at the speaker.
• Focus on what's important.
Many professions—from pilots to police officers—depend on keen powers of
observation. Training and practice help. But experts say things like chess and
videogames likely won't expand your overall attention skills.
• Allocate blocks of time to
specific tasks. Sometimes a deadline can force people to focus.
• Avoid distracted driving.
Don't talk on a cellphone, text or give voice commands while at the wheel.
No comments:
Post a Comment